Previous Page  28 / 100 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 28 / 100 Next Page
Page Background

28 United States

The Economist

May 5th 2018

1

2

a black teenager, as he was walking away.

The officer, Jason Van Dyke, who is about

to be tried for first-degree murder, had

been the subject of numerous complaints.

Changing such a culture will take time. In

Englewood, Mr Johnson tells his 350 offi-

cers to attend community meetings, to

build relationships and to avoid behaving

like an occupying force.

The riskwith policing software is that it

amplifies existing racial bias. “Technology

is far fromneutral,” says Kade Crockford of

the American Civil Liberties Union. when

police officers feed predictive policing al-

gorithms with their data on past stops and

arrests, so they can reinforce the bias that

police across the country stand accused of,

says Ms Crockford. For example, whites

and blacks consume and sell drugs at

pretty much the same rates, but far more

blacks are arrested for drugs thanwhites.

Used carefully, though, more data are

better than fewer, says Andrew Papachris-

tos of Northwestern University, and any-

wayHunchLab does not use arrest records.

It is too early to say whether the new tools

caused the decline in crime in Englewood

and other districts, though the evidence

suggests a correlation. This is good news

forMr Emanuel who is running for re-elec-

tion next year and is already facing a

crowded field of opponents. One of the

contenders for the city’s top job is Garry

McCarthy, whom Mr Emanuel sacked as

boss of the

CPD

in the wake of the Laquan

McDonald scandal. Mr McCarthy is likely

to run mainly on crime—until now, one of

Mr Emanuel’s biggest weak spots.

7

F

EDERAL Hill House is a squat building

in central Providence, within earshot of

the city’s main highway. On a recent rainy

Monday, a school holiday, the building

was full. Older children lounged in front of

a film, while toddlers roamed around the

soft play area. Some regularly spend more

than ten hours a day here, on top of school

hours, while their parents work. The chari-

ty provides essential support for low-in-

come families: it picks up children from

home before school starts, and looks after

them long after it ends. It accomplishes a

lot on a tight budget. In several places, the

ceiling lets through water from the grey

Rhode Island sky.

The youngest group of children at Fed-

eral Hill House are between18months and

five years old. There are 12 of them, with a

waiting list to join. The executive director,

Kimberly Fernandez, says some cannot

name any colours when they first arrive.

Some come to the centre hungry (it pro-

vides meals) or speaking no English. Oth-

ers arrive with behaviour problems. Par-

ents’ workschedules are often so inflexible

that Federal Hill must cover basic logistics

beyond school pick-up and drop-off. Ms

Fernandez says she had to use her own car

after some children took the wrong bus

home from school and wound up strand-

ed at the depot. Their mother was unable

to leavework to fetch them.

Plenty of evidence suggests that grow-

ing up poor, living through these kinds of

scrapes, has a detrimental impact on child

development. Children from rich families

tend to have better language and memory

skills than those from poor families. More

affluent children usually perform better in

school, and are less likely to end up in jail.

Growing up poor risks the development of

a smaller cerebral cortex. But these are as-

sociations between poverty and develop-

ment, not evidence that poverty causes

these bad outcomes, says Kimberly Noble,

a neuroscientist at Columbia University in

NewYork. She is part of a teamof research-

ers running a three-year experimentwhich

will, for the first time, search for causal

links between parental income level and a

child’s early development.

The teamwill start recruiting the first of

1,000 low-income mothers next week.

They will be invited to join the study,

which is called Baby’s First Years, shortly

after giving birth at one of ten hospitals in

four cities across theUnitedStates (to avoid

influencing the experiment, the research-

ers asked

The Economist

not to publish de-

tails about the cities). Of that 1,000,

roughly half will be randomly selected to

receive an unconditional $333 a month,

while the others will form a control group

that will receive $20. The money, which is

completely unconditional, will be loaded

onto a pre-paid debit card everymonth for

40months, on the date of the child’s birth-

day. The hypothesis is that this steady

stream of payments will make a positive

difference in the cognitive and emotional

development of the childrenwhose moth-

ers receive it.

The first data gathered will be baseline

interviews with the mothers just after re-

cruitment. This will reveal the various

backgrounds from which the mothers

come (all will have incomes below the

poverty line, roughly $23,000 for a family

of three). The researchers will conduct

phone interviews with all 1,000 mothers

around their child’s first birthday, thenvisit

them in their homes when their children

turn two. When they turn three, they will

be invited with their mothers to a research

lab in their city, where their child’s cogni-

tive skills will be tested and the electrical

activity of their brains studied.

Living experiment

The interviewswill alsomeasuremothers’

stress, mental health and employment pat-

terns. They will ask how the amount of

time mothers spend with their child is

changing, and gather data on the quality

and cost of child care and other child-relat-

ed expenses. The researchers will also

have a record of transactions made with

the debit card. The unconditional nature of

the cash transfer is inviolable: even if

mothers choose not to take part in the fol-

low-up studies, for which they are paid ex-

tra, they will still get the income for 40

months. The 1,000 mothers, minus poten-

tial dropouts, will provide enough statisti-

cal power to detect effects equivalent to

twomonths’ worth ofdevelopment in ear-

ly childhood, says GregDuncan, an econo-

mist on the team from the University of

California, Irvine.

A real-world experiment of this magni-

tude comeswith challenges. It has been six

years in the making, and the team has

spent years raising some $15m for it. About

$5.8m will be given away over the next

fouryears, towhichmust be added the cost

of recruiting and monitoring 1,000 people

over that time. The researchers worked to

get new legislation passed in two states in

which the experiment will be carried out,

in order tomake sure that those taking part

remain eligible for public benefits while

Child development

Mother’s money

PROVIDENCE, RHODE ISLAND

Researchers lookfor causal links between income and child development

Are we in the control group?

РЕЛИЗ ПОДГОТОВИЛА ГРУППА "What's News"

VK.COM/WSNWS